"WHAT IS CULTURAL PHYLOGENY, AND HOW DO WE RECOGNIZE IT?"
MICHAEL J. O'BRIEN
University of Missouri, Columbia, MO 65211 EEUU
Phylogeny refers to the genealogical history of any group of things, be they organisms, manuscripts, languages, or anything
else that changes over time by means of an ancestor passing on material to an offspring. Phylogeny should be an important
issue in both anthropology and archaeology because of their focus on history, that is, on questions about how and why people
and their cultural trappings change in certain ways over time. These are evolutionary questions, just as in biology questions
about organismal change over time are evolutionary. Not surprisingly, some of the methods that have been devised to examine
historical (evolutionary) questions in biology have significant value for the study of cultural phenomena. The transference
of methods from biology to anthropology is based on a growing recognition that artifacts, language, and other aspects of culture
are phenotypic features in the same way that shells, nests, and bones are phenotypic in the organismal world.
"NICHE CONSTRUCTION AND GENE-CULTURE CO-EVOLUTION"
JOHN ODLING-SMEE
Mansfield College, University of Oxford Oxford OX13TF,
United Kingdom
To varying extents all organisms choose, regulate, construct and destroy important components of their environments. In
doing so they modify natural selection pressures in their environments, and thereby introduce feedback to evolution. My colleagues
and I call these processes "niche construction". In humans, cultural processes greatly amplify our species's capacity
for niche construction, and human cultural niche construction is largely responsible for human material culture. I use gene-culture
co-evolutionary theory as a basis for exploring the contributions that cultural niche construction may have made to human
evolution in the past, and may still be making today.
SUBMITED PAPERS
THE TRADING ZONE IN EVOLUTIONARY ARCHAEOLOGY: AN ALTERNATIVE
TO A UNIFIED DARWINIAN PARADIGM
MARK ALDENDERFER Department of Anthropology University of Arizona Tucson,
AZ 85721-0030
Scientific knowledge of the past not only develops from unity, but also grows through diversity
and challenge. What appear to be incommensurate paradigms may at times be able to share data, concept, and method in creative
and useful ways. The "trading zone" a concept developed by philosopher of science Peter Galison, is the intellectual locus
in which competing paradigms may profitably benefit from one another. In this paper, I outline two domains of evolutionary
archaeology which may benefit from seeking partners in the trading zone: 1) learning processes and changes in attribute frequency;
and 2) parental investment and the locus of selection.
---------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
ARCHAEOLOGICAL THEORY AS AN EVOLUTIONARY PROCESS: DISTINGUISHING
FAD FROM THEORY AND WHY ANY UNIFIED PARADIGM IS IMPOSSIBLE
ALEX BENTLEY
Durham
University
In academic
settings that promote prestige rather than collective scientific progress, the individual incentive is to propose "new paradigms"
rather than develop existing theories. New theories are continually proposed before the old ones have had a chance to
be tested. Although academics themselves are under strong selection, the flux of new theories has come to resemble one
of two evolutionary processes, either: (1) random drift, or (2) selection, by which novelty rather than validity is the measure
of fitness. In either case, we can expect archaeological theory to change continually, like fashion, and
not to converge into a unified paradigm.
-----------------------------------------------------------
DARWINIAN EVOLUTIONARY INSIGHTS INTO POTTERY
AS AN ETHNIC MARKER
DAVID BULBECK
School of Archaeology and Anthropology, The Australian National University
In archaeology, the theoretical
explanation of variation in pottery decorations has focused on Information Theory, combined more recently with Agency Theory
(Bacus 2004). This paper considers whether the resulting insights can be cast within the framework of Darwinian cultural evolution,
examining the specific case of protohistoric and early historical pottery in Luwu, South Sulawesi, Indonesia (Bulbeck and
Caldwell 2000). A long-lived contrast between decorated pottery in the north and generally plain pottery along the central
coast correlates with different language stocks in these two areas. There are additional short-lived (c. 200 years) phenomena
of: (a) the dissemination of "soft pottery" associated with the establishment of a complex chiefdom in Luwu; and (b) idiosyncratic
decorative repertoires linked to immigrant groups. The predictions derivable from the hypotheses of biased transmission based
on similarity (Boyd and Richerson 2005: Chapter 6), and the niche-construction statements on cooperation and conflict (Odling-Smee
et al. 2003:298-301, 359-62), are tested against the Luwu data.
References:
Bacus, E.A. 2004. A consideration
of processes underlying Philippine pottery complexes. In V. Paz (ed.), Southeast Asian Archaeology: Wilhelm G. Solheim II
Festschrift, pp. 128-57. Quezon City: University of the Philippines Press.
Boyd, R. and P.J. Richerson. 2005. The
Origin and Evolution of Cultures. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Bulbeck, D. and I. Caldwell. 2000. Land of Iron:
The Historical Archaeology of Luwu and the Cenrana Valley. Results of the Origin of Complex Society in South Sulawesi Project
(OXIS). Hull: University of Hull Centre for South-East Asian Studies.
Odling-Smee, F.J., M.W. Feldman and K.N. Laland.
2003. Niche Construction: The neglected Process in Evolution. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
---------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
Explaining Variability
in the Importance of Agriculture, While Working Toward a Unified Darwinian Paradigm
MIKE CANNON
California State University, Long Beach
Some behavioral ecologists
have approached issues relating to the evolution of agriculture by using a model of optimal time allocation, which employs
average productive efficiency as a currency. I discuss an extension of this model
that incorporates risk, or variability in gross production, which has long been considered also to be relevant to the evolution
of agricultural economies. This model can be used to develop hypotheses about
the causes of variability in the importance of agriculture over space and time, and I discuss tests and potential tests of
these hypotheses. I conclude by addressing the relationship between HBE and selectionist
approaches to issues such as the development of agriculture, which not only are complimentary, but which must be integrated
in order to build complete evolutionary explanations.
-----------------------------------------------------------
TEMPORAL TRENDS IN THE MORPHOMETRIC VARIATION OF
THE LITHIC PROJECTILE POINTS DURING THE MIDDLE HOLOCENE OF SOUTHERN ANDES (PUNA REGION). A COEVOLUTIONARY APPROACH
MARCELO
CARDILLO
IMHICIHU-CONICET Instituto Multidisciplinario de Historia y Ciencias Humanas. DIPA. (Departamento de Investigaciones
prehistóricas arqueológicas).Saavedra 15. Buenos Aires, Argentina. ARG.
The focus of this paper is to discuss
theoretical and methodological issues regarding the study of artifact's morphometric variation, which to some extent can be
considered as an outcome of the cultural transmission process and replication error in the archaeological temporal and spatial
scales. The methodology proposed here, is based on the application of metrical and non metrical techniques, as geometric morphometrics.
The last one, allows us to perform shape and form analysis, treating these two variables independently one from the other.
This procedure is almost impossible with traditional morphometric methods. Even when these two elements of artifact's design
are highly related, it is analytically possible to discriminate each one, in order to search variation patterns related with
cultural learning. This approach could be very useful to expose the traits that were more controlled by learning rules. From
the analysis of an small sample of projectile points of the middle Holocene in the South Andes (Puna), we discuss here the
potentials and limitations of both methods: the traditional and the geometric morphometrics. Under the later, the performed
analysis suggest that while some basic traits of the form and shape have changed in different ways through time, other have
tended to endure. This pattern suggest a phylogenetic signal.
-----------------------------------------------------------
INTERDEMIC SELECTION AND PHOENICIAN PRIESTHOOD. DARWINIAN REFLECTIONS FROM THE SOUTHERN HISPANIC ARCHAEOASTRONOMY.
JOSE LUIS ESCACENA CARRASCO
Departamento de Prehistoria y Arqueología de la Universidad de Sevilla, España.
The most widespread interpretation sees in the religion a mechanism of reproduction of the society and of its
internal inequalities. The Marxist positions have reinforced this vision. But the above mentioned approach does not explain
why a conduct that only would benefit to the elites is present in all cultures. Also, for the general scorn of the historians
towards the biology as instrument for his investigations, the study of religion has not known the evolutionary mechanisms
linking faith and the reproduction of individuals and populations. From Darwinism I push back that the beneficiaries of
the religious credence are alone the highest classes, because the hierarchical structuring of a community is related to the
competition for the resources between groups, that is to say, for interdemic selection. I support, yet, that the Phoenicians
used his priests to organize their own dispersion. This way, they raised the foundation of new enclaves from the astronomical
knowledge of the clergy. Without its application, the emplacement of the colonies might have prevented from organizing the
commerce of overseas. This explains the link between foundational acts and sacred oracles. Such aspects are slightly studied,
especially for not interpreting many archaeological documents as cosmic symbols or astronomical knowledge. But there has not
been explained, especially, the interest of a community to nourish a subpopulation of memetic hipermutants (the priests) exempt
from any other obligation.
-----------------------------------------------------------
EVOLUTIVE CLADOGRAM
OF THE BULL SKIN MOTIVES AND ITS IMITATIONS IN THE ARCHAIC MEDITERRANEAN
JOSE LUIS ESCACENA CARRASCO*
DANIEL
GARCIA RIVERO*
*Departamento de Prehistoria y Arqueología de la Universidad de Sevilla,España.
Archaeology
works reconstructing ancient histories and processes, in many cases on the basis of a much reduced range of material culture.
Nevertheless, sometimes a bigger investment in research studies, which in many cases lead to an increase of data, is not correlated
with a better explanation of the archaeological problem. In part, this could be due to the insufficiency of the methods being
applied, specifically those which deal with the classification of archaeological materials. These and the chronological sequences
that archaeologists construct on the basis of these materials are very important in interpretative studies. That is why the
introduction of new methodological frameworks such as cladistics potentially allows us to open new perspectives and can help
us to tackle some of the common problems of the traditional classification systems. In this paper this method is applied to
some testimonies of the ancient Mediterranean -some of them with a ritual character- which seem to imitate the bull skin motive.
Thus, the first object is the assessment of the method itself, and the second, an exploration of the phyletic relationships
between these archaeological elements.
-----------------------------------------------------------
FROM
CLOVIS POINTS TO BANTU PEOPLES: WHAT DO DIFFERENT CULTURAL PHYLOGENIES MEASURE?
ETHAN E. COCHRANE
Institute
of Archaeology,University College London
31-34 Gordon Square,London WC1H 0PY.United Kingdom.
Defining
evolutionary relationships between populations is a primary goal of phylogenetic analyses in archaeology. With a phylogeny
in hand, scholars can develop explanations for particular patterns of descent, incorporating known chronological relationships
and diversity of taxa, as well as spatial, environmental, demographic and other associated population characteristics.
There
is, however, an important split among evolutionists pursuing phylogenetic explanations of cultural change: some examine artefact
classes, such as projectile point types, as phylogenetically related taxa, while others use cultures, often represented by
language groups, as taxa to be placed into a phylogenetic hierarchy. The artefact-type and culture-type approaches to phylogenetic
analysis make different assumptions about our ability to recognize human groups and how we measure transmission influenced
variation within and between them. Here I argue that for archaeological phylogenetic analyses, the artefact-type approach
is preferable when it includes methods linking the construction of artefact types to transmission processes and the definition
of human populations. Examples from ceramic research in Fiji exemplify the approach.
----------------------------------------------------------
TESTING MODELS OF EARLY PALEOINDIAN ADAPTATION USING BIOLOGICAL PHYLOGENETIC TECHNIQUES
MARK COLLARD*
BRIGGS BUCHANAN*
*Department of Anthropology and Sociology, University of British Columbia, 6303 NW Marine
Drive, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada, V6T 1Z1.
We examine Early Paleoindian technological traditions using cladistic
tree-building techniques from Biology to reconstruct a cultural phylogeny. Projectile point data collected from 26 Early Paleoindian
(ca. 11,500-10,500 BP) assemblages recovered from sites located across North America are used in the construction of cladograms.
Twelve quantitative, size-corrected characters are coded for cladistic analysis using the gap weighting coding technique.
Outgroup selection uses the oldest minimum radiocarbon age associated with an assemblage in the analysis. The resulting cladograms
are analyzed using permutation tail probability tests and goodness-of-fit statistics (consistency and retention indices) to
examine the degree to which the evolution of point form was a branching process or was blurred through horizontal transmission.
The results of this research, derived from a continental-scale database, allow questions concerning routes of migration into
North America, subsequent population movements and interaction, and adaptation to regional environments to be addressed.
-----------------------------------------------------------
PHYLOGENIES OF THULE HARPOON COMPONENTS FROM THE NORTH AMERICAN HIGH ARCTIC
JOHN DARWENT
Inglefield
Land Archaeology Project, Department of Anthropology,University of California, Davis, CA 95616-8522
The use of cladistics
for reconstructing phylogenies of archaeological phenomena is limited by scale. To demonstrate how scale, or the level of
the units of analysis, can affect phylogentic reconstruction I use late prehistoric- to historic-period Thule harpoons (ca.
A.D. 1100-1900) from the High Arctic of Alaska, Canada and Greenland as an example. A harpoon is a multi-component hunting
implement that typically consists of an endblade, harpoon head, foreshaft, socket, shaft, line, tension piece, and ice pick,
and was used primarily for sealing but also to hunt walrus and whale. Although these components are assembled into one harpoon,
and thus changes in one part might require changes in another, each component also can change separately from one another.
In other words, the components can evolve independently. Using High Arctic harpoons, this analysis underscores the necessity
of constructing phylogenies for the smaller-scale components of a composite artifact rather than at a whole-object level.
-----------------------------------------------------------
CULTURAL AND BIOLOGICAL PHYLOGENETICS WITHIN
POPULATIONS: WHAT CAN ONE PROFITABLY TEACH THE OTHER?
JOHN V. DUDGEON
University of Hawaii & IIRMES
We have been looking for specific analogues between
biological and cultural evolution for a long time. Early efforts at explaining the archaeological record using models constructed
from scientific evolutionary theory relied on adopted vocabulary and broad theoretical concepts, but rarely specified mechanisms
and linkages between theory and observation. Recent efforts to generate evolutionary accounts of the archaeological record,
while mechanistically sound, still struggle with units (what is being transmitted?), processes (how is it transmitted?), and
scale (how much transmission is visible?).
Although robust for explaining biological change, traditional population
genetics is ill-equipped to offer falsifiable explanations for cultural change because of manifold differences in fidelity
of transmission, and the probability of instantaneous recursion among interacting populations. Graph and network models of
transmission hold promise for delimiting analogous mechanisms of biological and cultural change. However, additional parameters
such as population-level spatial and compositional variation are necessary to instantiate the models and evaluate their empirical
sufficiency. From this basis, we can conceive of domains of variation common to both biological and cultural systems, and
evaluate the degree to which they match in terms of units, processes and scale. Such a model system and its implications for
geographic and demographic variability are outlined.
------------------------------------------------------------
A GROUP SELECTION MODEL OF TERRITORIAL WAR, XENOPHOBIA
AND ALTRUISM IN HUMANS AND OTHER PRIMATES.
AGNER FOG
Aalborg University Copenhagen,Denmark.
A
theoretical model of wars over group territories shows that behavioral traits like cooperative warfare, justice, altruism
and outsider exclusion may have coevolved in higher primates and prehistoric man. The conditions for territorial war to be
an effective mechanism of group selection are discussed. These conditions may have been present in tribal societies in prehistoric
times but not in modern times. The geographic evolution of territories is illustrated with a computer simulation.
-----------------------------------------------------------
CULTURAL TRANSMISSION, LINEAGES, AND THE EVOLUTION OF SOCIAL UNITS: AN EXAMPLE FROM THE HAWAIIAN ISLANDS
MICHAEL
W. GRAVES
University of Hawai'i
Americanist archaeologists have adapted the seriation method to an evolutionary
paradigm as a means to track cultural transmission. Cladistic analysis has been proposed for detailing cultural lineages.
How do we join these two approaches in an evolutionary archaeology? Differential transmission of stylistic traits in space
may lead to the evolution of distinct social groups; transmission of these same traits through time may result in related
cultural lineages. As a consequence, archaeologists can now describe the evolutionary history of artifact sets in terms of
the humans responsible for their creation. I describe an example from Hawai'i illustrating these methods with monumentally-scale
ceremonial sites (or heiau) and associated with chiefs within traditional Hawaiian society. It is now possible to identify
a cultural lineage linking chiefs who commissioned the construction of heiau at different monumental scales. At the same time,
heiau similarity can be used to measure the geographic scale of chiefly interaction through time. When both lineage and interaction
data are combined, the differential persistence of heiau and their associated leadership groups can be assessed in terms of
how widely they spread at the expense of others, the range of natural and social environments in which they came to occupy,
and how much labor could be garnered to support them.
-----------------------------------------------------------
TWO FACES OF DARWIN: THE COMPLEMENTARITY OF EVOLUTIONARY ARCHAEOLOGY AND HUMAN BEHAVIORAL ECOLOGY
KRISTEN
J. GREMILLION
The Ohio State University
Divergent positions on the application of Darwinism lie at the heart
of the divide between human behavioral ecology, with its focus on flexible responses to the environment, and evolutionary
archaeology, which analyzes cultural transmission over multiple generations. Although both approaches address all four of
Tinbergen’s types of explanation to some degree, behavioral ecology targets functional-adaptive causes of behavior,
whereas evolutionary archaeology emphasizes the evolutionary history of cultural traits. Evolutionary archaeology and archaeological
behavioral ecology share a fundamentally Darwinian set of core assumptions and both must contend with the limitations of an
incomplete evidential record. Both approaches have much to offer, and can coexist peacefully if their division of labor is
accepted and understood.
-----------------------------------------------------------
HEALTH OF NATIVE HUMAN
POPULATIONS IN AUSTRAL PATAGONIA, AN EVOLUTIONARY ECOLOGICAL PERSPECTIVE.
GUICHON R.A.(1,2)
M.H.FUGASSA (2)
J.A.SUBY (2)
R.CASALI(3)
(1) Conicet
(2)Depto. de Biología, Universidad Nacional de Mar del
Plata y Sede Quequén, Depto. de Arqueología, Universidad Nacional del Centro. Argentina. ARG.
(3)Fac. de Humanidades,
Universidad Nacional de Mar del Plata. Argentina. ARG.
The ecologic-evolutive approach turns health information
into an indicator of very diverse aspects referred to human societies. The presence of illnesses is, in general, the product
of unbalances between individuals and numerous environmental components. In this context, we started working two years ago
in the formulation of an investigation program, which has the evaluation of the biological impact of Native-European contact
in Southern Patagonia as its main concern. One of the objectives of this project is the search for a space for transversal
discussion of the biological, historical, archaeological an anthropological approaches. Therefore, from a paleoepidemiological
perspective, we are generating paleparasitological, nutritional paleopatologies and lifestyles information from human rests,
which come from diverse archaeological contexts and historical documentation. The aim of this presentation is to discuss the
possibilities and limitations that we have found when we approached the study of health in Southern Patagonia in the context
of an ecological-evolutive frame.
-----------------------------------------------------------
INVESTMENT,
COMPETITION, BET-HEDGING AND SIGNALING IN THE EVOLUTION OF EASTER ISLAND PREHISTORY
TERRY L. HUNT(1)
CARL
P. LIPO(2)
1)Department of Anthropology,University of Hawaii
2424 Maile Way Honolulu, HI 96817 USA
thunt@hawaii.edu
2)Department of Anthropology Institute of Integrated Research on Materials, Environments and Society (IIRMES)
California
State University Long Beach 1250 Bellflower Blvd
Long Beach, CA 90840
clipo@csulb.edu
Despite
its remoteness, small size, and marginal agricultural potential, Easter Island was the site of some of the greatest investments
in cultural elaboration anywhere in the ancient world. Such exuberant cultural phenomena contradict expectations of a surplus
production ("leisure time") hypothesis of the non-scientific orthogenetic paradigm. In contrast, we examine the developing
model of 'waste' in the evolution of populations in uncertain environments. For Easter Island we review our research on measuring
investment (e.g., in statues, roads, monumental architecture) and consider how a combination of bet hedging, sexual selection,
and costly signaling can robustly explain the level of intensity and form of cultural elaboration. Based on our continuing
work, we show that the record supports a short chronology for the island with massive investments occurring in the earliest
years following colonization. Over the centuries, a diversity of statue (moai) styles and materials converges into more standardized
forms, a pattern consistent with the consequences of costly signaling. Such signaling reflects competition through means other
than lethal warfare, as evidenced by the skeletal record and paucity of weapons. The record supports a relatively low population
size, an observation consistent with accounts by the first European visitors. After European contact, disease induced catastrophic
demographic collapse. Finally, we explore the expectations and implications of a potential sex ratio bias in the island population.
-----------------------------------------------------------
THE COGNITIVE DOMAIN OF ARTIFACTS: ONTOGENY, PHYLOGENY,
AND HUMAN CULTURAL LEARNING STRATEGIES.
KAREN JOHNSON
University of Michigan
Two features that
mark humans as a unique species are the creation of artifacts and processes of sociogenesis that sustain cultural knowledge
over time. This paper considers the architecture of the human mind that affords such features by reviewing current research
and theories in cognitive psychology. From here, this paper turns to a question of the relationship between ontogeny and phylogeny:
can the development of artifact understanding in children help inform an archaeological account of the emergence of artifacts
in hominid history? Though difficult, an appreciation of children's knowledge of artifacts may help in the attribution of
particular cognitive capacities of hominid species as seen through artifact types. Additionally, an ontogenetic perspective
provides insight into how human cultural processes -aided by the creation and organization of artifacts-operate on a time-scale
that is categorically different from evolutionary processes.
-----------------------------------------------------------
THE ARCHAEOLOGY OF ETHNIC BOUNDARIES AND MOSAICS: AN ECOLOGICAL AND EVOLUTIONARY PERSPECTIVE
SIBEL KUSIMBA
Northern Illinois University
Archaeology and anthropology have approached the problem of ethnic identities,
ethnic boundaries, and the relationship between ethnicity and material culture from a variety of perspectives. The work of
Barth is an explicitly ecological perspective on the problem of ethnic interactions and boundaries. This paper will evaluate
and critique the benefits and limits of ecological theory as an analog to understanding ethnic interactions, using archaeological
examples from Tsavo, Kenya and Mount Elgon, Kenya, two areas where multiethnic societies are reflected in the archaeological
and ethnographic records.
-----------------------------------------------------------
THE LAST FRONTIER. THE
PEOPLING OF SOUTH AMERICA FROM AN INVASIVE SPECIES PERSPECTIVE.
JOSE LUIS LANATA
CONICET- UBA, Argentina.
ARG.
In the human dispersal process, SouthAmerica is the last continental mass to which the Homo sapiens arrived.
Anthropology, linguistics, paleoanthropology and archaeology have discussed it from their own data. In our case we adopt an
invasive specie perspective to model tempo and mode of human dispersal using demographic models. South American peopling simulations
are presented based on the ecoregions available until 10 kybp, by considering a fast invasive process and a slow one. The
results show that the tempo of the dispersal would not be very different amongboth situations, producing the same metapopulations
under both circumstances. Comparing with the available archaeological data seems not to belong together the tempo of the dispersion
that is of the pattern, while the way of the same one agrees with the data paleontropológicos. We discuss our proposal in
connection with other available models.
-----------------------------------------------------------
MULTIDIMENSIONAL
SELECTION AND EVOLUTION: TOWARDS THE SCIENTIFIC STUDY OF DUAL-INHERITANCE, TRANSMISSION OF INFORMATION, MODULARITY, AND COMPLEXITY
AMONG HUMANS.
DANIEL O. LARSON
Department of Anthropology Environmental Science and Policy Institute
for Integrated Research in Materials, Environment, and Society, 1250 Bellflower Boulevard California State University,
Long Beach 90840-1003
Evolutionary processes have captured the attention of scholars from many disciplines for
over a hundred years. Common issues such as transmission, inheritance, selection, and selective constraints have frequently
challenged intellectuals to think 'outside the box.' To move forward as a science, anthropologists and archaeologists must
focus on careful use and consideration of borrowed terms and concepts. A common language with specific definitions is
fundamental to discourse and focused debate. Indeed, it is a critical concern for theory building as well as a practical issue
of operationalizing field and laboratory research programs. From an interdisciplinary perspective, I will examine terms and
concepts that can be employed to explore evolutionary events in the prehistory of the Anasazi of the American Southwest and
the Chumash of Coastal California. Previous notions regarding 'cultural complexity' are reevaluated from a scientific perspective
that rejects assumptions of evolution by human intentionality and directed variation. An archaeological science can be achieved
with careful integration of advanced evolutionary theory including key conceptual frameworks like modularity and multidimensional
selection. Contributions to an interdisciplinary dialogue about evolution can demonstrate the relevance of our discipline
to other natural and social sciences. In practical and real terms, archaeology must move from the dusty basement of intellectual
institutions to a new modern discipline leading the way to a better understanding of our universe.
-----------------------------------------------------------
THE CULTURAL PHYLOGENY OF MONUMENTAL STATUES ON EASTER
ISLAND
CARL P. LIPO (1)
TERRY L. HUNT (2)
1)Department of Anthropology Institute of Integrated
Research on Materials, Environments and Society (IIRMES),California State University Long Beach 1250 Bellflower Blvd,Long
Beach, CA 90840 clipo@csulb.edu
2) Department of Anthropology. University of Hawaii 2424 Maile Way, Honolulu,
HI 96817 USA thunt@hawaii.edu
The monumental statues (moai) of Easter Island represent significant investment
in cultural elaboration by the prehistoric islanders. Although statues have been the focus of great speculation and numerous
studies, we have remarkably little information about their chronology and degree of relatedness. Yet few studies could be
more relevant, as measurements of temporal and spatial statue variability are central to evolutionary models for explaining
cultural elaboration. In this paper we describe our use of a method based in cladistics and occurrence seriation to generate
a cultural phylogeny of the monumental statues. The method, based on graph theory, provides a means for measuring variability
over time and space, while making no assumptions about branching. In our study we seek to evaluate a model of statue change
in which diversity of styles and materials converges into more standardized forms, a pattern consistent with the consequence
of costly signaling. We present our results and discuss our efforts to test hypotheses using independent information.
-----------------------------------------------------------
THE STUDY OF THE ARCHAEOLOGICAL RECORD OF SANTA ROSA DE LOS PASTOS GRANDES, PUNA DE SALTA, ARGENTINA, FROM AN INCLUSIVE
EVOLUTIONARY PERSPECTIVE.
GABRIEL E. J. LÓPEZ
Grupo de Investigación Cultura, Comportamiento y Evolución
Humana (GICCEH.)Sección Arqueología, Universidad de Buenos Aires. CONICET,25 de Mayo 217 3º Piso, Buenos Aires, (1002), Argentina.
ARG.
Darwinian evolutionism presents different theoretical perspectives that can be applied to the study of the
archaeological record. In this work, particularly, the evolutionary ecology is emphasized, for being a theoretical frame that
allows to analyze human behavior from formal models, applicable to the interpretation of the archaeological evidence. The
case of study corresponds to an area of the high lands of the Northwest of Argentina, Puna of Salta, Pastos Grandes, which
average altitude is superior to 4000 m.snm. and the risk of unpredictable droughts is very high. The archaeological record
is represented by evidence of high and low density, so much in surface as in layer. From models of human behavioral ecology
, hypotheses of optimization and risk management are proposed for the study of archaeological materials, in special archaeofaunas
and lithics. Nevertheless, the social aspects that articulate human behaviors are not left of side. These are considered from
hypotheses derived from the mechanisms of the Theory of the Cultural Transmission. In this sense is emphasized the importance
of including different darwinian perspectives that allow to understand the variability of human behavior from the archaeological
record and different lines of evidence for the empirical application of this theoretical frame.
-----------------------------------------------------------
FROM LINNAEAN TAXONOMY TO A DARWINIAN PHYLOGENY OF PALAEOLITHIC TECHNOLOGIES: IMPLICATIONS FOR THE EVOLUTIONARY ANALYSIS
OF CULTURE
STEPHEN J. LYCETT
Leverhulme Centre for Human Evolutionary Studies, University of Cambridge, Fitzwilliam
Street, Cambridge, UK. E-mail s.lycett@human-evol.cam.ac.uk
The roots of an evolutionary approach to the archaeological
record can be traced to the 'culture historical' approaches of the early 20th century, despite the lack of a specific 'evolutionary'
theoretical or analytical framework being employed by these scholars. The relatively recent introduction of an evolutionary
theoretical framework for archaeological analysis explicitly transforms archaeological typologies from Linnaean taxonomies,
where evolution is not automatically implied, to Darwinian phylogenies, where issues of ancestry, descent, inheritance, transmission,
and the creation of lineages are brought to the fore. However, such shift in emphasis also creates a need for clear statements
regarding units of analysis (what is being transmitted and inherited?), what is evolving, and how these relate to populations
of artefact manufacturers. This paper will employ analysis of Palaeolithic technologies as a case study to examine these issues,
and considers the implications of transforming typological taxonomies into explicit phylogenies for existing archaeological
frameworks. The paper also reflects upon the implications of these analyses for issues relating to the patterns created by
phylogenetic analysis of technological units, and questions of 'culture' more generally.
-----------------------------------------------------------
EXTENDED PHENOTYPES, EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY, COMPLEX SYSTEMS, AND CULTURAL TRANSMISSION: WHY A UNIFIED DARWINIAN ARCHAEOLOGY
IS NOT ONLY DIFFICULT, BUT UNDESIRABLE AS WELL.
HERBERT D. G. MASCHNER*
BEN MARLER*
*Idaho
State University, Department of Anthropology, Campus Box 8005, Pocatello, Idaho 83209
We find it odd that
those who argue that artifacts are extensions of the human phenotype have never recognized that if this is true, then by default
it must affect the fitness of the manufacturer, not the fitness of the artifact. We find it surprising that those who study
the evolution of war have never recognized that the evolution of cooperation is the key evolutionary trait that made war possible,
and evolutionary psychology is critical to its understanding. It is further interesting that complex systems, the most analytical
and least humanistic of the potential Darwinian analyses, is now contributing more to our understanding of human behavior
than studies of kin-selection and evolutionary ecology. These discrepancies are interesting for one fundamental reason: while
Darwin's theory of evolution by natural selection is broad and encompassing, the use of that theory in archaeology has been
myopic and particularistic. Using examples from the Aleutian Islands of Alaska, we will demonstrate that the reason there
are so many different approaches to Darwinism in archaeology is not because we have different understandings of the concepts,
but because it is being applied to a suite of different problems that require different approaches. Ten years since the publication
of Darwinian Archaeologies, this is even truer than it was then.
-----------------------------------------------------------
EVOLUTIONARY PROCESS OF WARFARE AND MATERIAL CULTURE IN PREHISTORIC JAPAN.
TEKAHIKO MATSUGI**
*Institute
of Archaeology,University College London 31-34 Gordon Square. London WC1H0PY U.K.
*Department of Archaeology and
History,Faculty of Letters,Okayama University,3-1-1 Tsushimanaka,Okayama city 700-8530 Japan
We have plenty
of archaeological evidences related to warfare from the Yayoi period prehistoric Japan(10c.b.c-A.D.2c). These material representations
of dominance, the cardinal contents of agricultural 'package', are thought to have been brought by the immigrants from the
continent around 10c.b.c. They functioned as 'anchor' to transmit the dominant cultural complex both vertically and horizontally
to extend social stratification through the Yayoi era.
In the former and the middle phase of the Yayoi period(10-1c.b.c.),
this transmission operated vertically in main so that the material representations became more and more intensive and locally
particularized. This process is considered to have leaded the regionally specialized forms of social stratification based
on relatively staple population patterns.
To the contrary, the last phase of the Yayoi period(A.D.1-2c), with the
increasing fluidity of population owing to over-exploitation into the local lands and dependence on external resources such
as iron, saw the intensification of horizontal transmission of violent cultural complex. This resulted in the unification
of material representations for dominance, marking the emerging similarity of styles in stratified societies all over the
mainland Japan.
The author concludes that the sequence of local intensification and universal extension of social
stratification observed throughout the Yayoi period were generated by temporal change of population pattern dependent on enviromental
condition.
----------------------------------------------------------- THE EXPERIMENTAL SIMULATION OF ARCHAEOLOGICAL
PATTERNS: A CONTRIBUTION TO A UNIFIED SCIENCE OF CULTURAL EVOLUTION
ALEX MESOUDI
School of Psychology,
University of St Andrews.St Andrews, Fife KY16 9JP, Scotland, UK
In the last few years a number of evolutionary archaeologists
(e.g. Lipo et al., 1997; O'Brien & Lyman, 2000, 2003) have argued that certain archaeological patterns can be seen as
the result of the cultural transmission of information and artifacts from individual to individual within populations and
across successive generations. This has coincided with the development of experimental simulations of small-scale cultural
transmission by certain evolutionarily-minded anthropologists and psychologists (e.g. Baum et al., 2004; McElreath et al.,
2005; Mesoudi & Whiten, 2004). I shall argue that experimental methods such as these can potentially reveal important
insights into the large-scale patterns observed in the archaeological record. Experimental simulations offer a number of advantages
not available to archaeologists, such as the ability to 're-run' history more than once, the ability to isolate and control
single variables, and the generation of complete data-sets. Used in conjunction with archaeological methods and computer simulations,
such simulations can be used, for example, to support inferences regarding the precise cultural transmission mechanisms originally
responsible for generating different archaeological patterns. Finally, cross-disciplinary work such as this is facilitated
by the adoption of a unified Darwinian evolutionary approach to human culture (Mesoudi et al., 2004, submitted).
-----------------------------------------------------------
ARTIFACTS, BEHAVIORS AND MULTILEVEL SELECTION: A SYNTHETIC DARWINIAN PARADIGM IN EVOLUTIONARY ARCHAEOLOGY IS POSSIBLE
AND CONVENIENT. LESSONS FROM THE ARCHAEOLOGY OF THE ANDEAN PUNA OF ARGENTINA
HERNÁN J. MUSCIO
Grupo de
Investigación Cultura, Comportamiento y Evolución Humana (GICCEH).Sección Arqueología, Universidad de Buenos Aires. CONICET,
25 de Mayo 217 3º Piso, Buenos Aires, (1002) ARG.
Evolutionists always argued that artifacts and behaviors, in
a non metaphorical sense, are extensions of human phenotypes. Accordingly, evolutionary archeologists always considered individual
selection as one of many mechanisms to explain behavioral and technological evolution. But the dispersion of the several lines
of Darwinian research debilitates the emergence of a mature program in Evolutionary Archaeology. If archaeology takes as a
goal to contribute seriously in the empirical and theoretical domains to the scientific evolutionary research (as paleobiology
does), then a programmatic unification is required. Indeed, Evolutionary Archaeology is ready for that. Because Darwinian
evolution requires heritability, the evolutionary research mandates the exposure of cultural phylogenies and transmission.
The explanation of these phylogenies is a second step. At this instance, an unified Darwinian paradigm inclusive of the major
selectionist approaches to human culture and behavior is a promising source for hypothesis building and modeling, but only
if these lines are subordinated to the broader temporal and spatial scales of the archaeological phenomena. In the core of
the synthetic paradigm should stay the recognition of the theoretical nature of the evolutionary units. Indeed, artifacts
-at whatever scale of definition- can be sorted in evolutionary units, such as species and clades are, in order to study their
macroevolutionary dynamics. Also, is critical the recognition that human decision making processes, biased by genetic or cultural
evolved algorithms are selective in nature, and produce population level patterns, including the emergence of new selective
environments. Therefore, for the evolutionary archaeologists it is strategically convenient to abandon any unilevel reductionist
framework, in favor of a multilevel selectionist one. This should be oriented to explain the phylogenetic and the mechanist
sides of any evolutionary change archaeologically documented. This paradigm must be sensitive to the genetic and cultural
biases controlling human selective behavior at infraindividual levels, and to the ecological basis of the selective processes
at several scales, such as artifacts, individuals and groups. Also possible nested processes between different focal levels
require theoretical and empirical research. The synthetic paradigm in Evolutionary Archaeology should be one in which history,
as well as ecology, matters at properly archaeological scales. I exemplify this proposal discussing the ceramic evolution
of the early human food production niches from the Puna of Agentina. ----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
Unifying Darwinism
in Evolutinary Archaology
Hernán J. Muscio*
Gabriel E. López*
*Grupo de Investigación Cultura, Comportamiento
y Evolución Humana (GICCEH)
Sección Arqueología, Universidad de Buenos Aires.
CONICET
25 de mayo 217 3ºpiso, Buenos Aires (1002) ARG.
Darwinian evolution
is a theory about the effects of the inheritance of variation at populational levels. Cultural or biological evolutionary
entities are units subject to heritability, variation and replication. Natural selection is the basic mechanism of evolutionary
change, but not the unique. Asserting that artifacts and behaviors are part of the human phenotypes Evolutionary Archaeology
explains the archaeological record in terms of the Darwinian theory of evolution, including non adaptive evolutionary processes.
From Darwinism a great diversity of scopes has arisen, mainly from the approaches of the Human Behavioural Ecology, Sociobiology,
and the Cultural Transmission Theory. All these selectionist lines of reasoning
expand the explanatory domain of the evolutionary research in a variety of topics, including social, technological, and biological
evolution. However, a complete unified paradigm in Evolutionary Archaeology has not emerged.
In this paper we argue that the notion of artifacts and behaviours as extensions of the human phenotype, also capable
of being evolving units, gives the logical consistency to the unification of Darwinism in a single paradigm for Evolutionary
Archaeology. A Darwinian inclusive paradigm does not reduce theoretical diversity. Instead the explanatory potential of Evolutionary
Archaeology increases, complementing the historical side of Darwinism with hypotheses about changes affecting different aspects
of the human phenotypes at differing levels of evolutionary change. We illustrate these ideas discussing faunal consumption
and ceramic evolution of the early agropastoral occupations in the puna of Salta, Argentina.
----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
THE FRAMEWORK OF MODES OF EVOLUTION AND THE INTEGRATION OF HUMAN HISTORY WITHIN NATURAL HISTORY
ALBERT F. H. NACCACHE
Beirut, Lebanon
In this brief presentation I will try to show how the theoretical foundation of the framework
of Modes of Evolution (Naccache 1999) allows it to be used to analyze and organize archaeological data from the Lower Paleolithic
till the Historical period, and this in a non-reductionist way integrating Human History along the chain of emergences -from
atoms to populations-that characterizes our modern scientific worldview. I will emphasize two aspects: - How the approach
followed in the framework of Modes of Evolution (MoE), contrary to many of the 'isomorphic' approaches of Evolutionary Archaeology,
captures the 'emergent' character of Culture within the realm of Life. This will be done through the identification of the
MoE as major stages in the development of the mechanisms of transmission and reproduction of behaviors and artifacts in the
overall lineage leading to H. sapiens. - How the framework of MoE expands on the basic intuition of Maturana & Varela's
Autopoiesis (or today's fashionable 'Niche Construction'), by organizing archaeological data into a fine socio-cultural grid
that helps us describe and comprehend the emergence and development of specific human behaviors.
Naccache, A. F. H.,
1999, 'A brief history of evolution', History & Theory, 38:4, 10-32. -----------------------------------------------------------
ZAHAVI'S HANDICAP PRINCIPLE AND THE ORIGINS OF MESOAMERICAN
CIVILIZATION
HECTOR NEFF
IIRMES, California State University - Long Beach"
Fraser Neiman introduced
the evolutionary theory of signaling, originated by Zahavi, to Mesoamerican Archaeology in the mid-1990s, arguing that Maya
monuments could be understood as 'wasteful advertisements' and defining the Maya collapse as a shrinkage in the range of ability
to send costly (and thus reliable) signals. Independently, and apparently without any cross-fertilization from evolutionary
theorists, John Clark and his colleagues have argued that status competition involving altruistic acts drove the emergence
of ranked societies in eastern Mesoamerican during the Early Formative period. In this paper, the Mesoamerican 'mother
culture' itself (i.e., the Gulf Coast Olmec) is explained as a set of costly signals intended for different kinds of audiences.
Signals intended for local Gulf Coast people were borne on the famous 'colossal heads' and other stone sculpture of that region.
Signals intended for foreign audiences were borne initially on widely traded ceramic serving vessels, but, when the distinctive
iconographic system began to be copied, more-difficult-to-falsify jade became the preferred medium for long-distance signaling.
As a general principle for understanding communication between competing organisms, costly signaling would seem particularly
useful for archaeologists studying the elaborate monuments, architecture, and rare items of long-distance exchange that constitute
the trappings of 'complex society.'
-----------------------------------------------------------
EXPLORING THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN INDIVIDUAL AND GROUP SELECTION:
AN EXAMPLE FROM THE STUDY OF PRESTIGE GOODS.
AIMÉE PLOURDE
Centre for the Evolutionary Analysis of Cultural
Behavior, Institute of Archaeology, University College London.
Great gains have been made in evolutionary archaeology
through the employment of selectionist reasoning to examine behavior at the individual level, adopting insights from evolutionary
psychology and human behavioral ecology, and to understand the transmission of cultural variants. The study of selection at
the group level and the evolution of social institutions have also begun to benefit from adopting this perspective more rigorously.
However, while selectionist reasoning underlies all of these pursuits, a unified paradigm has not emerged in part because
it is often unclear how selection at each of these levels - the individual, the cultural variant, and the social group - impacts
or interacts with the others. I suggest that a closer examination of the connections between differing levels of selection
is needed. I will use the role of prestige goods in the formation of sociopolitical hierarchy as an example to illustrate
this point. I theorize that prestige goods' value arose from their initial function as honest (costly) signals of individual
skill or knowledge, in response to increasing levels of prestige competition in early human society. Prestige goods' function
at the individual level then provided the mechanism for the role that they later played in group selection.
-----------------------------------------------------------
NICHE CONSTRUCTION APPLIED: TRIPLE-INHERITANCE INSIGHTS INTO THE POST-GLACIAL RE-COLONIZATION OF SOUTHERN SCANDINAVIA
FELIX RIEDE
McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research University of Cambridge
Darwinian Archaeology
today is in a difficult, but potentially rewarding situation: Strongly criticized by many archaeologists and ignored by most
biologists, evolutionary-minded archaeologists have to juggle and reconcile two diametrically opposed views of the world.
However, some biologists acknowledge that cultural behaviour can have significant long-term effect on the biological make-up
of a species and the 'niche construction' or triple-inheritance model has been designed to integrate, on an even footing,
the biological, ecological and cultural dynamics of human evolution. The niche construction approach has inherent value
to Darwinian archaeologists as it embraces not only technological but also ecological aspects of human evolution. Furthermore,
it acknowledges the impact of the transmission of social knowledge on other levels of selection (i.e. the ecological and the
genetic). The niche construction model has been applied to a case study of the pioneer post-glacial re-colonization of Southern
Scandinavia (c. 15 - 13kya). I argue that while it is perhaps important to think about a 'unified paradigm' for Darwinian
Archaeology it is as important if even more so to demonstrate to other archaeologists as well as to biologists that archaeology
has much to say regarding the evolution of modern human biological and cultural diversity.
-----------------------------------------------------------
ENVIRONMENTAL CHANGE, HUMAN PREDATION AND FORAGING EFFICIENCY
IN THE CENTRAL HILLS OF ARGENTINA.
DIEGO RIVERO*
MATÍAS E. MEDINA*
*Cátedra de Prehistoria y Arqueología
(Universidad Nacional de Córdoba)- CONICET.Pabellón Argentina-Cara Sur,Ciudad Universitaria - Córdoba (Capital)5000 Córdoba
- Argentina. ARG.
The maintenance of subsistence system based on high ranking and gregarious artiodactyls but,
delimited to upper mountain grassland range are evaluated in this paper.A simulation model with two significant prediction
is presented: a) because of biogeoghraphical isolation this resource are highly sensitive to human predation; and b) a narrow
diet based on big game cannot be maintenable beyond Early Holocene (10000-6000 BP). To test this hypothesis archaeofaunal
evidence procceding from four Holocene Central Hills (Argentina) archaeological sites is analized. Some of them have been
excavated by the authors -Arroyo El Gaucho 1 (Pampa de Achala, Córdoba) and Puesto La Esquina 1 (Pampa de Olaen, Córdoba)-
but assemblages from classical archaeological sites of the region -Intihuasi (Sierras de San Luis, San Luis) and Ongamira
(Sierras Chicas, Córdoba)- were reanalyzed too. According to the Diet Breath Model higher ranking resources will be exploited
providing they are encounter. Consequently, it is assumed that its frequency in archaeological record reflects its abundance
in local landscape. This way, the temporary variability in taxonomic abundance, richness, age profile and processing intensity
marks along the Holocene are analyzed. To finish, the results are related to other archaeological evidence indicating
changes in mobility, technology, increase importance of gathering and the establishment of agricultural practices on the Late
Holocene.
----------------------------------------------------------- COGNITION AND COMMUNICATION IN THE ACHEULEAN
OF THE HUNSGI-BAICHBAL BASIN, KARNATAKA, INDIA.
CERI SHIPTON
Centre for Human Evolutionary Studies (LCHES),
University of Cambridge
Following the premise of the Social Brain Hypothesis, that there is a fundamental link
between sociality and cognition in humans, it is proposed that the social interaction is the smallest unit of transmission
for the evolution and ontogeny of cognition and culture. This study aims to identify aspects of Acheulean sociality and cognition
and patterns therein. Experimental, core flaking data and spatial patterning are used to demonstrate systematic reduction
strategies at Isampur Quarry, indicative of the propensity of Acheulean hominins to imitate one another. Using a microscribe
digitizer, three-dimensional morphological data is collected on c. 500 bifaces from 10 sites in the Hunsgi-Baichbal Basin.
Site-wise standardised forms of Biface morphology are used to determine degrees of relatedness among sites. To determine
biface refinement, variables relevant to the hominins and their goals in biface manufacture are identified. Site-wise differences
in biface refinement and raw material transfer distances, illustrate differences in dexterity and planning capacity among
hominins, which in turn have implications for linguistic abilities. It is proposed that the homogeneity of the Acheulean
masks great changes in communication and cognition among Pleistocene hominins, only subtlely manifested in lithic technology.
----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
darwinian
dynamics in cultural skills and cultural forms
Monica
Tamariz
Language Evolution and
Computation Research Unit,
The University of Edinburgh.
I propose a double selection
system to study cultural phenomena. In system I, cultural skills (like the ability to produce and understand language, art,
technology or social behaviour) develop in people’s brains through repeated exposure to units of replication (e.g. speech
patterns, techniques, features of social interaction) that are probabilistically encoded in cultural behavior. These units
of replication are transmitted when the behavior is performed and the next generation of humans learn them.
In system II, cultural
forms (like speech utterances, art objects, pieces of technology or instances of social interaction) develop from the information
contained in units of replication (concepts) that exist in people’s brains. Concepts are transmitted (via the forms)
to other people’s brains during single instances of cultural interaction.
The two systems cannot
exist without each other. Development of a concept into a form (system II) takes place thanks to the cultural skill that maps
concept onto pieces of behaviour (system I). Skills (system I) would not exist in the first place if there were no concepts
(system II) to be mapped onto behavior. And culture (systems I and II together) must increase human fitness, so natural selection
can favor the genes that provide culture with the neural substrate it needs. ----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
CLADISTIC AND ETHNOGRAPHIC ANALYSES OF CRAFT TRANSMISSION
IN RURAL IRAN: TOWARDS AN INTEGRATED STUDY OF CULTURAL DIVERSITY AND CHANGE
JAMSHID TEHRANI *
MARK COLLARD
**
* Centre for the Evolutionary Analysis of Cultural Diversity, University College London, UK
**Department
of Anthropology and Sociology, University of British Columbia, Canada
Anthropologists and archaeologists have
a long-standing interest in the evolution of cultural diversity and how it relates to linguistic, genetic and geographical
patterns. Current debates regarding these issues have focused on the extent to which cultural assemblages arise by ancestral
populations splitting into new ones ('phylogenesis') or through borrowing and blending among neighbouring populations ('ethnogenesis').
Here, we aim to further advance empirical research in this field by reporting the results of our recent case study of craft
transmission in rural Iran. The case study investigated relationships among different tribal groups'craft traditions by
employing a novel and experimental combination of research methods adopted from evolutionary biology and social anthropology.
A cladistic analysis of textile-related craft traits indicated that both branching and blending influenced the evolution of
Iranian tribal craft traditions. This mixed pattern conformed to the ethnographic data on craft learning gathered during fieldwork
in rural Iran, which indicated that although weavers initially learn their skills from their mothers, many subsequently adopt
traits from external sources through a variety of media. However, subsequent analyses demonstrated that the latter do not
have a uniform impact on the transmission of all Iranian craft traits, and vary in different regions and historical periods.
|